Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
2019, Applied Neuropsychology: Child
Spatial orientation is an important function in daily life because it allows us to reach a target place when moving through our environment, using self-centered (egocentric) or environmental information (allocentric). Compared to other cognitive functions, spatial orientation has been studied less in preschool ages. Some brain areas, such as the hippocampus and the temporal as well as the parietal and frontal cortices, are involved in spatial orientation. Therefore, when these brain regions are altered in neurological conditions or in atypical development in children, we would expect impairment of spatial abilities. The aim of this study is to review studies, published in recent years, that use egocentric and allocentric spatial orientation tasks for assessing spatial memory in preschool children, with the final goal of finding out which tests could be included in a clinical neuropsychological evaluation. We observed that although egocentric spatial orientation emerges first during development, allocentric spatial orientation tasks are employed at very early ages. Most of these tasks are performed in real environments, allowing children's self-movements and using environmental modifications, but technologies such as virtual or augmented reality are increasingly used. Other aspects are discussed, such as the lack of consensus in the nomenclature, the difficulty of tracing the course of development of spatial orientation, or the ecological validity of the tests used. We finally observed that there is greater interest in studying the allocentric framework than the egocentric one, which makes it difficult to compare the use of the two frames of reference during a neuropsychological evaluation in preschool-aged children.
Nutrients
Various lifestyle factors, including diet, physical activity, and sleep, have been studied in the context of children’s health. However, how these lifestyle factors contribute to the development of cognitive abilities, including spatial cognition, remains vastly understudied. One landmark in spatial cognitive development occurs between 2.5 and 3 years of age. For spatial orientation at that age, children learn to use allocentric reference frames (using spatial relations between objects as the primary reference frame) in addition to, the already acquired, egocentric reference frames (using one’s own body as the primary reference frame). In the current virtual reality study in a sample of 30–36-month-old toddlers (N = 57), we first demonstrated a marginally significant developmental shift in spatial orientation. Specifically, task performance with allocentric performance increased relative to egocentric performance (ηp2 = 0.06). Next, we explored a variety of lifestyle factors, includ...
Brain and Behavior, 2020
This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
The spatial orientation ability is very important for peoples' everyday life, as well as for their distinction to the sector of athletics, which depends on many endogenous and extraneous factors. The aim of present research was to study the effect of gender and athletic occupation in the orientation ability of children (boys and girls) of age 4 to 5 years old. On this survey participated 400 children who were evaluated with the "Witeba-Test of Spatial Orientation, (Temple, Williams and Bateman, 1979)". Data were analyzed with 2-way ANOVA (gender x athletic occupation). The results revealed nonsignificant differences between boys and girls, between the children that did or did not participate in sports and no interaction either (p > 0.05). By inference, from the present study it appeared that the ability of orientation consists an inherent feature which at least during the early years of children's development, is not affected by the above factors.
Developmental Psychobiology, 1984
In order to test the spatial competence of preschool infants, two groups, aged 2 years and 4 years, were tested on a version of the "radial maze" task. They were required to obtain a chocolate sweet from each of eight identically labelled positions in an unfamiliar room. Four-year-olds accomplished this with considerable accuracy, rarely revisiting previously sampled positions. Two-year-olds performed marginally above chance. Variants of the task were employed, including rotation of the room configuration so that labelled positions were rendered ambiguous with respect to all other environmental stimuli. The latter caused a marked fall in performance in those infants that had been scoring above chance. By these criteria, infants clearly possess a form of intuitive "spatial memory" that is likely to mature during the second and third years. Parameters of performance were remarkably similar to those seen in comparable studies with nonhumans. It is likely that hippocampal development in early infancy underlies this evolving skill, assuming cross-species similarity in the organization of spatial behavior, and that locomotor competence is required for its development.
Developmental Science, 2011
Although there is much research on infants' ability to orient in space, little is known regarding the information they use to do so. This research uses a rotating room to evaluate the relative contribution of visual and vestibular information to location of a target following bodily rotation. Adults responded precisely on the basis of visual flow information. Seven-month-olds responded mostly on the basis of visual flow, whereas nine-month-olds responded mostly on the basis of vestibular information, and 12month-olds responded mostly on the basis of visual information. Unlike adults, infants of all ages showed partial influence by both modalities. Additionally, 7-month-olds were capable of using vestibular information when there was no visual information for movement or stability, and 9-montholds still relied on vestibular information when visual information was enhanced. These results are discussed in the context of neuroscientific evidence regarding visual-vestibular interaction, and in relation to possible changes in reliance on visual and vestibular information following acquisition of locomotion. The contribution 3 The contribution of visual and vestibular information to spatial orientation by 6-to 12-month-old infants and adults When adults move around the world, they are adept at maintaining a sense of their position in space despite extensive bodily displacements and reorientations. It is easy to overlook this process, given its frequently effortless nature. But it is clearly a fundamental component of spatial processing and, in addition to reaching an understanding of its basis in adults, it is important to understand its developmental origins from infancy onwards. Typically, research on infant spatial orientation investigates infants' ability to relocate a target following some form of bodily movement and/or reorientation. Several researchers have investigated the conditions under which infants relocate a fixed target after a bodily rotation. One method (Cornell
1982
A program of research was conducted to study transitions from rpreoperational to concrete operational'forms of spatial imagery (area 1), to compabre results from spatial imagery studies based on open-ended me6sures (such as drawings) with results based on reaction time measures (area 2), and to study anticipatory imagery in the contexts of memory apd problem-formulative ,anticipation (area 3). Research in area 1 tested three predictions, generated f-rom a revision of Piagetian theory, concerning children's performances on two anticipatory imagery tasks and a standard. conservation task. Discussion in area 2 reports results of a Study testing the'hypothesis that dLawing errors on anticipatory kinetic ,nriagery ta.sks reflect children's poor images of objects in anticipated states of movement. Also reviewed is a .study comparing preschool and older children's abilities to mentally track an abject through a rotation movement, as well as further investigations addressing questions of developmental differences under certain task conditions, age differences in mental tracking strategies, and the relationships of strategies to tracking rates. Risearch in area 3 investigates whethei children mentally transform objeot states on tasks in which particular processing strategies are unspecified and examines the effect of transforming strategies on short-;term and long-terM memory for figurative states. Related materials are appended. (RH) * Reproductions supplied by EDRS axe the best that can be rd'ade * * from the original document.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2001
Mobile organisms can keep track of spatial location (both their own location and that of objects in the environment) using either an external referent system or one centered on the self and updated by information about movement through space. When the latter system is disabled (e.g., by rapid turning), aspects of the external world must be used to reestablish orientation. Recently, it has been claimed that, both for rats and for human toddlers, reorientation is achieved using a geometric module that accepts only information about the metric properties of the environment (C.
PLOS ONE, 2016
This paper presents the MnemoCity task, which is a 3D application that introduces the user into a totally 3D virtual environment to evaluate spatial short-term memory. A study has been carried out to validate the MnemoCity task for the assessment of spatial short-term memory in children, by comparing the children's performance in the developed task with current approaches. A total of 160 children participated in the study. The task incorporates two types of interaction: one based on standard interaction and another one based on natural interaction involving physical movement by the user. There were no statistically significant differences in the results of the task using the two types of interaction. Furthermore, statistically significant differences were not found in relation to gender. The correlations between scores were obtained using the MnemoCity task and a traditional procedure for assessing spatial short-term memory. Those results revealed that the type of interaction used did not affect the performance of children in the MnemoCity task.
Developmental Psychobiology, 2010
Three experiments are described that investigate 4.5-month-old infants' spatial thinking during passive movement using a task that required no manual or visual search. In these experiments, infants habituated to a display located near one corner of a table. Before the test trial the infants were either moved to the opposite side of the table or they remained in the same position that they held during the habituation trials. Also, between the habituation trials and the test trial, the display was either surreptitiously moved to the diagonally opposite position on the table, or the display remained stationary. The results showed that infants generally dishabituated when the actual (allocentric/objective) location of the display was changed between habituation and test. However, in Experiment 3, in which infants had reduced experience moving around the testing chamber, infants dishabituated to a change in their egocentric spatial relationship to the display. The results of this experiment suggest that experience moving around the testing chamber was a prerequisite for such location constancy. Taken together, the findings presented here indicate that with enough experience, young infants become aware of key spatial relationships in their environment during passive movement.
2020
The impact of development and healthy aging on spatial cognition has been traditionally attributed to a difficulty in using allocentric strategies and a preference for egocentric ones. An alternative possibility, suggested by our previous works, is that this preference is actually conditioned by the spatial cues (e.g. geometric of landmark cues) present in the environment rather than a strategic choice per se. We tested this prediction by having 79 subjects (children, young and older adults) navigating a Y-maze composed either of landmarks or geometric cues, with an immersive head-mounted display that allows us to record both head and eye movements. Our results show that when the performance is based on landmarks solely, children and older adults exhibit a deficit in using allocentric strategies when compared to young adults. Hence, an inverted U-profile of allocentric strategies was observed across the lifespan. This was not due to a default of attention to the landmarks, as eviden...
Frontiers in Developmental Psychology, 2015
Spatial navigation is an adaptive skill that involves determining the route to a particular goal or location, and then travelling that path. A major component of spatial navigation is spatial reorientation, or the ability to reestablish a sense of direction after being disoriented. The hippocampus is known to be critical for navigating, and has more recently been implicated in reorienting in adults, but relatively little is known about the development of the hippocampus in relation to these large-scale spatial abilities in children. It has been established that, compared to school-aged children, preschool children tend to perform poorly on certain spatial reorientation tasks, suggesting that their hippocampi may not be mature enough to process the demands of such a task. Currently, common techniques used to examine underlying brain activity, such as electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), are not suitable for examining hippocampal development in young children. In the present paper, we argue for the use of eyeblink conditioning (EBC), a relatively under-utilized, inexpensive, and safe method that is easy to implement in developing populations. In addition, EBC has a well defined neural circuitry, which includes the hippocampus, making it an ideal tool to indirectly measure hippocampal functioning in young children. In this review, we will evaluate the literature on EBC and its relation to hippocampal development, and discuss the possibility of using EBC as an objective measure of associative learning in relation to large-scale spatial skills. We support the use of EBC as a way to indirectly access hippocampal function in typical and atypical populations in order to characterize the neural substrates associated with the development of spatial reorientation abilities in early childhood. Thus, we advocate for EBC as a simple biomarker for success in various tasks that require the hippocampus, including spatial reorientation.
Previous spatial cognition studies have shown that young children tended to rely on information about the macroscopic shape of the environment to reorient themselves, whereas they ignored the non-spatial feature information. The present study investigated disoriented four-, five-and six-year-olds searching for a toy in a small rectangular arena, where the spatial layout was manipulated according to geometric properties (sense and length). The results show that children as young as 4 years are able to reorient above chance using wall colour and length together, when not required to rely on the left/right judgement. Furthermore, the evidence we found concerning the relationship between spatial language production and reorientation performance suggests that linguistic abilities might play an important role in enabling older children to build stronger spatial representation combining the available properties of an enclosed space.
Brain Imaging and Behavior, 2019
The ability to orient and navigate in spatial surroundings is a cognitive process that undergoes a prolonged maturation with progression of skills, strategies and proficiency over much of childhood. In the present study, we used functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) to investigate the neurological mechanisms underlying the ability to orient in a virtual interior environment in children aged 10 to 12 years of age, a developmental stage in which children start using effective spatial orientation strategies in large-scale surroundings. We found that, in comparison to young adults, children were not as proficient at the spatial orientation task, and revealed increased neural activity in areas of the brain associated with visuospatial processing and navigation (left cuneus and mid occipital area, left inferior parietal region and precuneus, right inferior parietal cortex, right precentral gyrus, cerebellar vermis and bilateral medial cerebellar lobes). When functional connectivity analyses of resting state fMRI data were performed, using seed areas that were associated with performance, increased connectivity was seen in the adults from the right hippocampal/parahippocampal gyrus to the contralateral caudate, the insular cortex, and the posterior supramarginal gyrus; children had increased connectivity from the right paracentral lobule to the right superior frontal gyrus as compared to adults. These findings support the hypothesis that, as children are maturing in their navigation abilities, they are refining and increasing the proficiency of visuospatial skills with a complimentary increase in connectivity of longer-range distributed networks allowing for flexible use of efficient and effective spatial orientation strategies.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 2001
Research with both rats and human infants has found that after inertial disorientation, the geometry of an enclosed environment is used in preference over distinctive featural information during goal localization. Infants (Homo sapiens, 18-24 months) were presented with a toy search task involving inertial disorientation in 1 of 2 conditions. In the identical condition, 4 identical hiding boxes in a rectangular formation were set within a circular enclosure. In the distinctive condition, 4 distinctive hiding boxes were used. Infants searched the goal box and its rotational equivalent significantly more than would be expected by chance in the identical condition, showing that they were sensitive to the geometric configuration of the array of boxes. Unlike the results of studies using a rectangular enclosure, however, in the distinctive condition, infants searched at the correct location significantly more than at other locations.
Visual Cognition, 2007
In a series of experiments we tested 4-and 8-month-olds' ability to represent the spatial layout of an object across changes in its orientation with respect to egocentric spatial coordinates. A fixed-trial familiarisation procedure based on visual habituation behaviour shows that both age-groups are able to discriminate between different object-centred spatial configurations.
Canadian Journal of Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie, 1982
The speed with which spatial information is classified into categories was tested by asking 129 subjects from four age groups-34 six-year-olds, 35 ten-year-olds, 30 fourteen-year-olds, and 30 adult university students-to sort six decks of cards, each requiring a spatial judgment. The results indicated that: (1) oblique discriminations required more processing time than did vertical-horizontal discriminations; (2) stimuli along the vertical axis were not processed significantly more quickly than stimuli along the horizontal axis; and (3) position variations slowed orientation discriminations, especially if the lines were of oblique orientation. The effect of position variations was particularly marked for the 6-year-olds. The role of positional cues in spatial organization, as well as aspects of developmental changes in spatial concepts and strategies, are discussed. RESUME Evaluation de la vitesse de categorisation de I'information spatiale chez 129 Ss de quatre groupes d'age-34 Ss de 6 ans, 35 Ss de 10 ans, 30 Ss de 14 ans et 30 Ss adultes de niveau universitaire-qui avaient a classifier six ensembles de cartes requerant chacun un jugement spatial. Les resultats montrent que (1) la discrimination des obliques exige un temps de traitement plus long que celle des verticales et des horizontales; (2) les stimulis disposes selon un axe vertical ne sont pas traites plus rapidement que ceux qui sont disposes selon un axe vertical; (3) les variations de position ralentissent les discriminations d'orientation, plus specialement dans le cas ou les lignes sont orientees obliquement. L'effet des variations de position a ete particulierement marque chez les enfants de 6 ans. La discussion porte sur le role des indices de position dans I'organisation spatiale de meme que sur certains aspects des changements lies au developpement dans les concepts et les strategies relatifs a I'espace. Spatial displays are often analyzed by means of the perceptual features and dimensions that underly them (Gibson, 1969). Thus, how space is organized and processed may be understood in the context of spatial features such as position (i.e., up, down, right, left) and orientation (i.e., vertical, horizontal, oblique). Reports by Appelle (1972), Ibbotson and Bryant (1976), Laurendeau and Pinard (1970), and others have documented the finding that vertical orientation serves as a prime dimension for guiding perception, and that within this dimension, "up" serves as the unmarked (positive) pole
Infant Behavior and Development, 2006
An appreciation of object-centred spatial relations involves representing a 'within-object' spatial relation across changes in the object orientation. This representational ability is important in adult object recognition . Recognition-bycomponents: A theory of human image understanding. Psychological Review, 94, 115-147;. Representation and recognition of the spatial organisation of three-dimensional structure.
Cognitive Psychology, 2013
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution and sharing with colleagues.
Spatial memory is the metaphorical hook on which our everyday experiences hang. One way of thinking about the development of spatial memory is as a progression from (a) egocentric (self-based) codings to (b) simple allocentric (world-based) codings relative to just one spatial reference to (c) codings using multiple allocentric spatial references together. We re-examined that timeline in a simplified virtual environment. Children aged 3-5 years saw a virtual penguin in relation to symmetric landmarks and then recalled its location after being ‘teleported’ to a new viewpoint. We found that children aged 3.5-4.0 years successfully used a single-landmark recall strategy, which is even younger than our previous work with a more cue-rich environment (Negen, Heywood-Everett, Roome & Nardini, 2017). Children succeeded at multiple-landmark allocentric recall at age 4.0-4.5 years, which is also younger than our previous findings in a more cue-rich environment. In addition, an inhibition measure was found to be a significant predictor of single-landmark strategies and recall accuracy. These findings suggest that single- and multi-landmark allocentric recall emerge earlier than previously thought, and that executive functions may form a bottleneck in environments that are not purposefully simplified to have fewer cues than naturalistic environments.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Navigation in a complex environment can rely on the use of different spatial strategies. We have focused on the employment of ''allocentric" (i.e., encoding interrelationships among environmental cues, movements, and the location of the goal) and ''sequential egocentric" (i.e., sequences of body turns associated with specific choice points) strategies during navigation. To investigate the developmental pattern of these two strategies in school-aged children, we used a virtual reality paradigm in which the spontaneous or imposed use of both strategies could be assessed. Our results showed an increase in spontaneous use of the allocentric strategy and also an increase in reliance on environmental landmarks with age. Although a majority of the children spontaneously used the sequential egocentric strategy, all age groups performed above chance when the allocentric strategy was imposed. Altogether, our findings suggest that young children are able to employ an allocentric strategy but that the nature of this allocentric strategy changes progressively in a complex cognitive representation between 5 and 10 years of age.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.