Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
The use of family history as a predictive tool in medicine is widely accepted, yet its significance is overstated for non-genetic diseases. While a small percentage of diseases are caused by inheritable genetic mutations, the vast majority—estimated at 90–95%—are driven by lifestyle and environmental factors. This article critically examines the assumption that familial disease history indicates individual risk, arguing that cultural, environmental, and technological changes across generations render many such comparisons misleading and inaccurate. It highlights how inherited behaviors, such as diet or clothing habits, may masquerade as genetic inheritance. The role of negative expectation and belief in shaping health outcomes is also considered, as fear induced by medical predictions and testing for familial diseases can itself be detrimental. The article questions the commercial motivations that often accompany tests and the resulting preventive treatments, which can last a lifetime. Ultimately, it calls for a shift away from genetic fatalism and toward individual agency, cultural context, and personal responsibility in health.
Qualitative Health Research, 2007
In this study, the authors sought to examine how risk information is articulated in relation to health problems that people identify as personally important and relevant. The respondents were receptive to health education messages, using different types of information in relation to its personal relevance and as a resource for managing and exercising control over perceived risk. People were not fatalistic about disease risk, as reported in previous research. Instead, they were responsive to complex public health messages and actively engaged in rationalizing their health risks, although this did not necessarily result in behavioral change. Consequently, a theoretical distinction exists between taking responsibility for evaluating complex public health messages and taking responsibility for behavioral change. The authors conclude that people's rationalizations about health risks often mirror the medical model of disease, suggesting that they are responsive to, and not fatalistic toward, such public health information.
BMC Public Health, 2010
Background: A family history, reflecting genetic susceptibility as well as shared environmental and behavioral factors, is an important risk factor for common chronic multifactorial diseases such as cardiovascular diseases, type 2 diabetes and many cancers.
American Journal of Health Education, 2007
Advances in the fi eld of human genomics have important implications for the prevention of chronic disease. In response to these advancements, public health professionals-including health educators-must become competent in the principles underlying the interface between genomics and the use of family health history. Family health history captures the familial nature and incidence of chronic diseases and provides valuable insights on the risk for chronic diseases within the context of shared genes, environments, and behavior. The purpose of this article is to review family health history research as an important tool for assessing chronic disease risk; to provide information regarding its use in health education practice as a potential preventive tool; and to discuss the ethical, legal, and social implications of such use.
Sociology of Health and Illness, 1994
Recent advances in molecular genetics are making it increasingly feasible to constmct individual genetic profiles predicting susceptibility to heart disease, cancer and respiratory disorders. This paper reviews current knowledge about the social and cultural impact of providing people with information relating to their risk for future disease, focusing not only on currently available predictive genetic testing but also on hypertension, hyperlipidaemia and cancer screening. We highlight the importance of issues of probability and uncertainty, and the tension between collective and individual goals in the assessment of medical risk. We conclude with a proposed research agenda for studies of the social and cultural impact of predictive genetic testing, and argue that there is a pressing need for rigorous, empirical, social research in this area.
Journal of epidemiology and community health, 2015
The Annals of Family Medicine, 2004
PURPOSE Although the family history is increasingly used for genetic risk assessment of common chronic diseases in primary care, evidence suggests that lay understanding about inheritance may confl ict with medical models. This study systematically reviewed and synthesized the qualitative literature exploring understanding about familial risk held by persons with a family history of cancer, coronary artery disease, and diabetes mellitus.
Psychology & Health, 2010
Journal of Risk Research, 2017
The prevailing scientific approach to genetic risk information centres around communication of risk in terms of numerical probabilities. However, it is well known that individuals have difficulties in understanding and making sense of this information in their own lives. There is, accordingly, a need to investigate whether any methodologies in psychological research may shed light on how individuals perceive genetic risk information within their specific contexts of family history, personal relationships, lifestyles and future plans. To explore whether hermeneutic phenomenology and methodology may offer a deeper understanding of an individual's perception of having a hereditary predisposition, we conducted a literature search. We found that Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis may be a fruitful approach to an individual's lived experiential world. The studies analysed showed how individuals interpret information about genetic risk in the light of their own beliefs about the multiple causes of illness, patterns of heredity and observable risk factors in their families. People's understanding of their experience is derived from an intricate interconnectedness with others that arises in the context of a world shaped in equal measure by language and culture, on the one hand, and bodies and objects on the other.
Sociology of Health & Illness, 1999
This paper reports the findings of an interview study of women (n = 40) attending genetic counselling for hereditary breast/ ovarian cancer (HBOC). The analysis indicates that women who attend genetics clinics perceive themselves as having a responsibility to their kin (past, present and future generations) to establish the magnitude of their risk and the risks to other family members, and to act upon this information by engaging in some form of risk management. It is observed that in acknowledging their genetic responsibility for their kin these women not only relinquished their right not to know about their risks, but also committed themselves to undertaking risk management practices which may have iatrogenic consequences. It is argued that the construction of genetic risk as a moral issue can be seen as limiting the choices which are available to women who attend genetic counselling.
New Genetics and Society, 2010
NIH consensus and state-of-the-science statements, 2009
Institutes of Health consensus and state-of-thescience statements are prepared by independent panels of health professionals and public representatives on the basis of 1) the results of a systematic literature review prepared under contract with the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ); 2) presentations by investigators working in areas relevant to the conference questions during a 2-day public session; 3) questions and statements from conference attendees during open discussion periods that are part of the public session; and 4) closed deliberations by the panel during the remainder of the second day and morning of the third. This statement is an independent report of the panel and is not a policy statement of the National Institutes of Health or the U.S. government. The statement reflects the panel's assessment of medical knowledge available at the time the statement was written. Thus, it provides a "snapshot in time" of the state of knowledge on the conference topic. When reading the statement, keep in mind that new knowledge is inevitably accumulating through medical research. Many common diseases have genetic, environmental, and lifestyle antecedents that family members share, and health care professionals in the United States have long used family history information as a risk assessment tool. In addition, most hereditary diseases have been elucidated through the study of families. A person's family history has the potential to capture information about shared factors that contribute to risk for common diseases, such as diabetes, stroke, cancer, and heart disease. Family history is also used routinely in many other ways, including its welldefined use in determining who might benefit from genetic testing and its use in the interpretation of genetic test results. The combination of these attributes makes the systematic collection of family history a potentially important step in personalizing health care. Several tools are in development to allow family history information to be effectively incorporated into health information technology systems, including electronic health records, personal health record systems, and family history risk assessment tools. Understanding the scientific foundation of family history is important if clinical decision aids (based on the information) are to be useful to clinicians and persons in typical practice settings and in improving clinical outcomes. Although the term family history is commonly used, it does not have a common definition-that is, various clinicians and patients understand it differently. Available family history questionnaires include information about a wide range of genetic, social, cultural, and environmental factors. Furthermore, family history questions may be embedded in complex risk assessment tools that incorporate many other demographic and health factors. Moreover, the definition of family varies when viewed from the perspectives of geneticists, generalist and specialist clinicians, family therapists, and members of some ethnic and cultural groups. The accuracy of patient-provided information is limited by a person's awareness, understanding, recollection, and willingness to disclose health issues of family members. The expected use of information from family history and the ex
Annual Review of Public Health, 2010
This report describes the use of information emerging from genetic discovery to motivate risk-reducing health behaviors. Most research to date has evaluated the effects of information related to rare genetic variants on screening behaviors, in which genetic risk feedback has been associated consistently with improved screening adherence. The limited research with common genetic variants suggests that genetic information, when based on single-gene variants with low-risk probabilities, has little impact on behavior. The effect on behavioral outcomes of more realistic testing scenarios in which genetic risk is based on numerous genetic variants is largely unexplored. Little attention has been directed to matching genetic information to the literacy levels of target audiences. Another promising area for research is consideration of using genetic information to identify risk shared within kinship networks and to expand the influence of behavior change beyond the individual.
Genetics in Medicine, 2010
for the Family Healthware TM Impact Trial (FHITr) Group Purpose: To determine whether family medical history as a risk factor for six common diseases is related to patients' perceptions of risk, worry, and control over getting these diseases. Methods: We used data from the cluster-randomized, controlled Family Healthware TM Impact Trial (FHITr). At baseline, healthy primary care patients reported their perceptions about coronary heart disease, stroke, diabetes, and breast, ovarian, and colon cancers. Immediately afterward, intervention group participants used Family Healthware TM to record family medical history; this web-based tool stratified familial disease risks. Multivariate and multilevel regression analyses measured the association between familial risk and patient perceptions for each disease, controlling for personal health and demographics. Results: For the 2330 participants who used Family Healthware TM immediately after providing baseline data, perceived risk and worry for each disease were strongly associated with family history risk, adjusting for personal risk factors. The magnitude of the effect of family history on perceived risk ranged from 0.35 standard deviation for ovarian cancer to 1.12 standard deviations for colon cancer. Family history was not related to perceived control over developing diseases. Risk perceptions seemed optimistically biased, with 48 -79% of participants with increased familial risk for diseases reporting that they were at average risk or below. Conclusions: Participants' ratings of their risk for developing common diseases, before feedback on familial risk, parallels but is often lower than their calculated risk based on family history. Having a family history of a disease increases its salience and does not change one's perceived ability to prevent the disease. Genet Med 2010:12(4): 212-218.
Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 2010
Increased availability of genetic risk information may lead the public to give precedence to genetic causation over behavioral/environmental factors, decreasing motivation for behavior change. Few population-based data inform these concerns. We assess the association of family history, behavioral risks, and causal attributions for diseases and the perceived value of pursuing information emphasizing health habits or genes. 1,959 healthy adults completed a survey that assessed behavioral risk factors, family history, causal attributions of eight diseases, and health information preferences. Participants' causal beliefs favored health behaviors over genetics. Interest in behavioral information was higher than in genetic information. As behavioral risk factors increased, inclination toward genetic explanations increased; interest in how health habits affect disease risk decreased. Those at greatest need for behavior change may hold attributions that diminish interest in information for behavior change. Enhancing understanding of geneenvironment influences could be explored to increase engagement with health information.
American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 2009
Health Psychology, 2009
Objective: Genetic tests vary in their prediction of disease occurrence, with some mutations conferring relatively low risk and others indicating near certainty. The authors assessed how increments in absolute risk of disease influence risk perceptions, interest, and expected consequences of genetic tests for diseases of varying severity. Design: Adults (N ϭ 752), recruited from New Zealand, Australia, and the United Kingdom for an online analogue study, were randomly assigned to receive information about a test of genetic risk for diabetes, heart disease, colon cancer, or lung cancer. The lifetime risk varied across conditions by 10% increments, from 20% to 100%. Main Outcome Measures: Participants completed measures of perceived likelihood of disease for individuals with mutations, risk-related affect, interest, and testing consequences. Results: Analyses revealed two increment clusters yielding differences in likelihood perceptions: A "moderate-risk" cluster (20%-70%), and a "high-risk" cluster (80%-100%). Risk increment influenced anticipated worry, feelings of risk, testing-induced distress, and family obligations, with nonlinear patterns including disproportionately high responses for the 50% increment. Risk increment did not alter testing interest or perceived benefits. These patterns of effects held across the four diseases. Conclusion: Magnitude of risk from genetic testing has a nonlinear influence on risk-related appraisals and affect but is unrelated to test interest.
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 2003
Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 2008
To evaluate the effect of the genetic risk information source (family history vs genetic test results) on parents' concern about their own and their children's genetic disease risk. Design: Randomized trial using a Web-enabled survey.
Journal of Immunology, 2003
The aim of this article is threefold. First, we describe the accuracy of people's risk perception who have been screened on familial hypercholesterolemia (FH) in a family-based screening program. Second, we identify factors that modify risk perception. Finally, we show the influence of risk perception on subsequent preventive behavior. The risk perception of 556 screenees (677 participants, overall response = 82%) was measured by postal questionnaires on three occasions: at screening and 3 days and 7 months after the test result was reported to the patient. Presentation of the risk was precategorized and given both as numerical (1 in x) and as verbal probability. In addition, medication use and attitudes toward gene therapy were determined 7 months after screening. On average, the screenees underestimated their numeric risk of having FH and getting a myocardial infarction (MI). Furthermore, FH-positive screenees perceived that they were at greater risk of MI than FH negatives, and screenees with the highest actual risk used medication more, perceived a greater risk, and opted more often for future gene therapy. Risk perception of having FH was influenced by cholesterol level, while MI risk perception was affected by age, education, cholesterol level, and cardiovascular disease (CVD) in the family. We conclude that FH-positive screenees correctly perceive a higher risk of getting a heart attack than do FH-negative screenees. Screenees did not believe that MI was inevitable, and risk perception was associated with both medication use and the intention to opt for gene therapy, but not with other preventive measures. Thus, genetic risk notification seems to be acceptable and does not lead to aversion to preventive behavior. © 2002 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.