Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
2017, Frontiers in psychology
This study examines how the domains of reward and attention, which are often studied as independent processes, in fact interact at a systems level. We operationalize divided attention with a continuous performance task and variables from signal detection theory (SDT), and reward/aversion with a keypress task measuring approach/avoidance in the framework of relative preference theory (RPT). Independent experiments with the same subjects showed a significant association between one SDT and two RPT variables, visualized as a three-dimensional structure. Holding one of these three variables constant, further showed a significant relationship between a loss aversion-like metric from the approach/avoidance task, and the response bias observed during the divided attention task. These results indicate that a more liberal response bias under signal detection (i.e., a higher tolerance for noise, resulting in a greater proportion of false alarms) is associated with higher "loss aversion.&...
2015
This study examines how processes such as reward/aversion and attention, which are often studied as independent processes, in fact interact at a systems level. We operationalize attention with a continuous performance task and variables from signal detection theory, and reward/aversion with a keypress task using variables from relative preference theory. We find that while the relationship between reward/aversion and attention is functionally invariant, a power law formulation akin to the Cobb-Douglas production function in economics provides the best model fit and theoretical explanation for the interaction. These results indicate that a decreasing signal-to-noise with signal detection results in higher loss aversion. Furthermore, the estimated exponents for the multiplicative power law suggest capacity constraints to processing for attention and reward/aversion. These results demonstrate a systemic interaction of attention and reward/aversion across subjects, with a quantitative s...
Experimental Psychology, 2004
How does the perceptual system process stimuli that signal aversive outcomes or dangers? Considering the functional links between perception, attention, and action regulation, we posit that when people can avoid the aversive consequences, sensitivity of the perceptual system to danger signals should be enhanced, whereas it should be reduced when there is no such option. To test this prediction, we used a search task in which tachistoscopically presented conjunctions of features had to be detected. Parameters of sensitivity and response bias were analyzed drawing on procedures from signal detection theory. Although the experimental procedure rewarded correct responses, the predicted asymmetry emerged. For stimuli that were linked to a negative consequence (loss of points in the experimental game), perceptual sensitivity was enhanced when participants had the opportunity to neutralize the loss in a second task; an opposite pattern emerged when they had no such opportunity.
2011
Abstract 1. Research suggests that threatening information captures attention more rapidly than neutral information. However, in most studies threat stimuli differ perceptually from neutral stimuli and are instrumental to perform the task, leaving the question unanswered whether threat is sufficient to capture attention. In experiment 1, we designed a visual search task with stimuli of equal salience (colored circles) that have the potential to lead to efficient search (10 ms/item).
Social Cognition, 2005
Two experiments investigated the influence of approach/avoidance-related motor actions on attention allocation to affective stimuli. Employing a modified visual search paradigm, Experiment 1 demonstrated that orientation-incongruent stimuli have a stronger attention grabbing power than orientation-congruent stimuli. Using a dual-task paradigm, Experiment 2 extended these findings by showing that incongruency effects in attention allocation mirror differences in the attentional resources required to encode affective information, such that orientation-incongruent stimuli require more attentional capacity than orientation-congruent stimuli. Interestingly, effects were generally stronger for positive as compared to negative stimuli, and for approach-as compared to avoidance-related motor actions. Functional and theoretical implications are discussed.
PsycEXTRA Dataset, 2000
2009
Abstract Many biological models of human motivation and behavior posit a functional division between those subsystems responsible for approach and avoidance behaviors. Gray and Mc-Naughton's (2000) revised Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) casts this distinction in terms of a Behavioral Activation System (BAS) and a Fight-Flight-Freeze System (FFFS), mediated by a third, conflict resolution system-the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS). They argued that these are fundamental, functionally distinct systems.
Emotion, 2008
The authors show that predispositions to approach and avoid do not consist simply of specific motor patterns but are more abstract functions that produce a desired environmental effect. It has been claimed that evaluating a visual stimulus as positive or negative evokes a specific motor response, extending the arm to negative stimuli, and contracting to positive stimuli. The authors showed that a large congruency effect (participants were faster to approach pleasant and avoid unpleasant stimuli, than to approach unpleasant and avoid pleasant stimuli) could be produced on a novel touchscreen paradigm (Experiment 1), and that the congruency effect could be reversed by spatial (Experiment 2) and nonspatial (Experiment 3) response effects. Thus, involuntary approach and avoid response activations are not fixed, but sensitive to context, and are specifically based on the desired goal.
Learning & Behavior, 2010
It has long been acknowledged that discrimination training of the kind AX BX results not only in the stimuli involved acquiring different levels of associative strength, but also in changes in the attention they are paid. Over 50 years ago, Lawrence (1949) proposed that this arrangement results in stimuli that are relevant to the discrimination being paid more attention than are irrelevant stimuli. The discrimination above, where A is relevant because it consistently signals reinforcement (S) and B is relevant because it consistently signals nonreinforcement (S), raises the question of whether attention to the former, to the latter, or to both stimuli will be enhanced by this training. At a theoretical level, the answer to this question would appear to be that attention to both cues will be enhanced. Sutherland and Mackintosh (1971) proposed that if a stimulus successfully predicts the outcome of a trial, attention to that stimulus and all other exemplars from the same dimension will increase. Since both S and S meet this requirement, there should be an increase in attention to both. Mackintosh (1975) proposed that attention will increase to a stimulus that is the best predictor of the trial outcome and will generalize to other, similar stimuli. Although the trial outcome was generally regarded as being the presence, rather than the absence, of reinforcement, Mackintosh (1975, p. 288; see also Le Pelley, 2004) acknowledged that his proposals could equally well apply to stimuli that are the best predictors of nonreinforcement. Although there is a growing body of evidence to suggest that attention will be high to stimuli that consistently signal reinforcement (Pearce & Mackintosh, 2010, provide a recent review), there is less evidence showing that attention to stimuli that consistently signal nonreinforcement will also be high. The purpose of the two experiments reported here, therefore, was to determine whether more attention is paid to a stimulus that is a reliable signal for nonreinforcement than to an irrelevant stimulus. If changes in attention occur during discrimination learning, they should be reflected in new learning when the stimuli are used in a different task. However, it is necessary to show that any apparent differences in the associability of these stimuli (the ease with which they are subsequently learned about) are not simply the result of associations acquired during the original training. This problem has been tackled in two main ways. The first involves training subjects, after an initial discrimination has been acquired, with the same stimuli but a different response requirement. In an early experiment of this kind, Lawrence (1949) trained rats to choose between two arms of a maze, which differed in size, brightness, and texture. Only one of these dimensions was relevant, such that approaching one cue (S) from this dimension was reinforced but approaching the other (S) was nonreinforced. Subjects were then transferred to a new task where turning left in the presence of one of the cues from the original training and right in the presence of the other cue from the same dimension was reinforced. Lawrence found that the new discrimination was acquired more readily when the cues had been relevant, rather than irrelevant, for the first discrimination. Because the acquisition of the responses for the new discrimination was intended to be unaffected by the associations formed during the initial training, it was concluded that the original training resulted in more attention being paid to relevant than to irrelevant stimuli. However, it is not clear from Lawrence's result whether attention was enhanced to both relevant cues (S and S) during the original training or just to S .
2007
The dot probe task is often used to assess attentional bias in anxiety, but some aspects need clarification. First, the results, which are traditionally summarized in an attentional bias index, do not allow for distinguishing between different selective attention processes; orienting and disengagement. Second, different versions of the dot probe task have been used with unknown relative merits. Participants ðN ¼ 133Þ completed two versions of the dot probe task: the detection task (i.e. is there a probe?) and the differentiation task (i.e. what sort of probe is it?). The analysis carried out allowed for a differentiation between orienting and disengagement. The main finding was that trait anxiety is related to disengagement difficulties and not to speed of orienting. Concerning the relative merits of the two dot probe task versions, the results suggest that the detection task may be superior to the differentiation task. Implications for past and future research are discussed. r
Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 2006
The influence of a peripheral cue represented by a gray ring on responsivity to a subsequent target varies. When a vertical line inside a ring was a go target and a white small ring inside a ring was a no-go target, reaction time was shorter at the same location relative to a different location. However, no reaction time difference between the two locations occurred when a white cross inside the ring, instead of the white vertical line inside the ring, was the go target. We investigated whether this last finding was due to a forward masking influence of the cue, a requirement of low attention for the discrimination or a lack of attention mobilization by the cue. In Experiment 1, the intensity of the cue was reduced in an attempt to reduce forward masking. In Experiment 2, the vertical line and the cross were presented in the same block of trials so as to be dealt with a common attentional strategy. In Experiments 3 and 4, the no-go target was a 45º rotated cross inside a ring to increase the difficulty of the discrimination. No evidence was obtained that the cross was forward masked by the cue nor that it demanded less attention to be discriminated from the small ring. There was a facilitation of responsivity by the cue when the small ring was replaced by the rotated cross. The results suggest that when the discrimination to be performed is too easy the cue does not mobilize attention.
Cognition and Emotion, 2001
Current research on automatic attention allocation focuses on the questions whether there is an asymmetry in attentional biases towards negative and positive stimuli and whether these attentional biases are influenced by situational variables. In an experiment with N = 48 participants, automatic allocation of attention to chance and danger stimuli was investigated. Attentional capture was generally larger for chance stimuli than for danger stimuli. Additionally, attentional bias was influenced by the outcome focus of the actual goal orientation. Results revealed an incongruence effect of goal orientation on attentional biases: Attentional capture for the chance and danger stimuli was comparatively stronger when an outcome focus of opposite valence had been induced.
2010
In attentional bias modification programs, individuals are trained to attend away from threat in order to reduce emotional reactivity to stressful situations. However, attending towards threat is considered to be a prerequisite for fear reduction in other models of anxiety. We compared both views by manipulating attention towards or away from an acquired signal of threat. The strength of extinction and reacquisition was assessed with threat and US-expectancy ratings.
The present experiment examined the effects of varying stimulus disparity and relative punisher frequencies on signal detection by humans. Participants were placed into one of two groups. Group 3 participants were presented with 1:3 and 3:1 punisher frequency ratios, while Group 11 participants were presented with 1:11 and 11:1 punisher frequency ratios. For both groups, stimulus disparity was varied across three levels (low, medium, high) for each punisher ratio. In all conditions, correct responses were intermittently reinforced (1:1 reinforcer frequency ratio). Participants were mostly biased away from the more punished alternative, with more extreme responses biases found for Group 11 participants compared to Group 3. For both groups, estimates of discriminability increased systematically across the three disparity levels and were unaffected by the punisher ratios. Likewise, estimates of response bias and sensitivity to the punisher ratios were unaffected by changes in discriminability, supporting the assumption of parameter invariance in the Davison and Tustin (1978) model of signal detection. Overall, the present experiment found no relation between stimulus control and punisher control, and provided further evidence for similar but opposite effects of punishers to reinforcers in signal-detection procedures.
Cognition & Emotion, 2011
Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 2018
Over the past decade an increasing number of studies across a range of domains have shown that the repeated performance of approach and avoidance (AA) actions in response to a stimulus leads to changes in the evaluation of that stimulus. The dominant (motivational-systems) account in this area claims that these effects are caused by a rewiring of mental associations between stimulus representations and AA systems that evolved to regulate distances to positive and negative stimuli. In contrast, two recently forwarded alternative accounts postulate that AA effects are caused by inferences about the valence of actions and stimuli (inferential account) or a transfer of valenced action codes to stimulus representations (common-coding account). Across four experiments we set out to test these three competing accounts against each other. Experiments 1-3 illustrate that changes in stimulus evaluations can occur when people perform valenced actions that bear no relation to a distance regulat...
Perception & Psychophysics, 1981
A behavioral model for performance on signal-detection tasks is presented, It is based on a relation between response and reinforcement ratios which has been derived from both animal and human research on the distribution of behavior between concurrently available schedules of reinforcement, This model establishes the ratio of obtained reinforcements for the choice responses, and not the probability of stimulus presentation, as the effective biaser in signal-detection research, Furthermore, experimental procedures which do not control the obtained reinforcement ratio are shown to give rise to unstable bias contours. Isobias contours, on the other hand, arise only from controlled reinforcement-ratio procedures,
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
The present studies aimed to understand how approach and avoidance states affect attentional flexibility by examining attentional shifts on a trial-by-trial basis. We also examined how a novel construct in this area, task context, might interact with motivation to influence attentional flexibility. Participants completed a modified composite letter task in which the ratio of global to local targets was varied by block, making different levels of attentional focus beneficial to performance on different blocks. Study 1 demonstrated that, in the absence of a motivation manipulation, switch costs were lowest on blocks with an even ratio of global and local trials and higher on blocks with an uneven ratio. Other participants completed the task while viewing pictures (Studies 2 and 3) and assuming arm positions (Studies 2 and 4) to induce approach, avoidance and neutral motivational states. Avoidance motivation reduced switch costs in evenly proportioned contexts, whereas approach motivation reduced switch costs in mostly global contexts. Additionally, approach motivation imparted a similar switch cost magnitude across different contexts, while avoidance and neutral states led to variable switch costs depending on the context. Subsequent analyses revealed that these effects were driven largely by faster switching to local targets on mostly global blocks in the approach condition. These findings suggest that avoidance facilitates attentional shifts when switches are frequent, whereas approach facilitates responding to rare or unexpected local stimuli. The main implication of these results is that motivation has different effects on attentional shifts depending on the context.
Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.